UNCOVERING CLUES TO THE NEUROBIOLOGIC
BASIS OF EMOTION AND
CONSCIOUSNESS
BOSTON—Once considered too
intangible to study, the mysteries of emotion and
consciousness are now beginning to be unraveled with
neuroimaging techniques and molecular biology. "Rather than
being elusive, emotion is as much amenable to scientific study
as any other aspect of the mind," said Antonio R. Damasio, MD,
PhD, Professor and Head of the Department of Neurology at the
University of Iowa College of Medicine, Iowa City, and Adjunct
Professor at the Salk Institute, in La Jolla, California.
During the 125th Annual Meeting of the American Neurological
Association, Dr. Damasio and other researchers in the field of
cognitive neuroscience discussed the roles of emotion, memory,
and consciousness in the definition and management of various
neurologic and psychiatric disorders.
THE SEARCH FOR THE EMOTION LOCUS
According to
Dr. Damasio, emotion is an "expression of basic mechanisms of
life regulation developed in evolution and is indispensable
for survival." The neural systems related to emotion appear to
be selective, he noted. Patients with bilateral amygdala
damage have been shown to have difficulty processing fear and
anger, and they cannot recognize these emotions in others.
These patients, he added, have no difficulty with other
emotions.
With the
advent of positron emission tomography (PET), researchers
began looking for the physical substrates of emotion and
feeling. When subjects were asked to imagine and reenact
emotional states, there were "profound changes" in the PET
images, Dr. Damasio said. Feeling the emotion of sadness, for
instance, resulted in bilateral insular cortex activation and
partial activation of the cingulate gyrus. Feeling happiness
activated the insula cortex in a slightly different pattern
and also activated the posterior cingulate and secondary
somatosensory cortex. Feeling fear resulted in deactivation of
secondary somatosensory regions, he said.
DISORDERS OF CONSCIOUSNESSŠ
"Consciousness is in the direct line of succession
from emotions and feeling," Dr. Damasio proposed. He referred
to consciousness as a combination of two processes. One
process can be thought of as the "movie in the brain" that
spatially and temporally integrates the pattern of mental
images of varied sensory types. The other process is the
"self" that senses that the movie happens to be yours. The
movie is shot in "your perspective" and "you can act on its
contents as agents." In this context, he said that patients
with akinetic autism, absence seizures, global amnesia, and
coma provide prime examples of disorders of consciousness.
The
distinction between "self" and "others" can be considered a
specific neuronal task that, when altered, can lead to
pathologic states, said John C. Mazziotta, MD, PhD, Professor
of Neurology, Radiological Sciences, and Molecular and Medical
Pharmacology and Director of the Brain Mapping Center at the
University of California, Los Angeles.
"We
constantly monitor and imitate the actions of others," Dr.
Mazziotta said. Indeed, mirror neurons respond when a subject
performs a complex motor task or watches another individual
performing the same task. Shared motor representations may
allow us to develop empathy and to learn languages, accents,
and social gestures, he said. When a subject looks at a person
who has an expression of sadness, for example, the subject
preactivates his or her own muscles that express
sadness.
However, the
discovery of mirror neurons has led to further questions about
how we retain a sense of self. Proprioception, Dr. Mazziotta
observed, seems to be a crucial component in this distinction.
Studies of patients with dorsolateral prefrontal lesions, he
continued, suggest that this region may be required to
suppress the automatic mimicking of others. Therefore,
"pathologic aberrations of this inhibition may result in
symptoms associated with echolalia, echopraxia, autism, and
schizophrenia," he said.
SUBCONSCIOUS COMPUTATIONS
Although the
brain performs a vast number of neuronal computations, few
result in conscious awareness, Dr. Mazziotta said. A recent
imaging study suggested that visual awareness is the result of
subconscious computations. According to the study, viewing an
ambiguous figure results in competing percepts within the
brain. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, the
researchers captured a salient state in which higher-order
visual areas were activated while the primary visual cortex
was deactivated. Although Dr. Mazziotta acknowledged that
current imaging techniques have limited spatial and temporal
resolutions, they can be used to begin exploring the nature of
consciousness.
DECISION-MAKING DEVICE
"The highly
structured nervous system serves as an exquisite
decision-making device," said Michael S. Gazzaniga, PhD,
Director of the Center for Cognitive Neuroscience at Dartmouth
College, Hanover, New Hampshire. "From perception to rational
choice, the brain functions automatically even as we struggle
to believe we freely will our own actions," he
said.
Studies of
split-brain patients have yielded insights into the
organization of the sensory and motor systems and, perhaps,
clues to the nature of conscious experience. Although the left
hemisphere is specialized for language, speech, and major
problem-solving capacities and the right hemisphere is
specialized for tasks such as facial recognition and
attentional monitoring, processes that require activities in
both hemispheres seem totally integrated, Dr. Gazzaniga said.
Even when the corpus callosum is severed, and the two
hemispheres act independently, "we believe that we are in
charge of our actions," he said. The rules of human nature, he
suggested, are "enacted before personal conscious awareness."
According to Dr. Gazzaniga, "these phenomena appear to be
related to our left hemisphere's interpreter, a device that
allows us to construct theories about the relationship between
perceived events, actions, and feelings."
MEMORY, LEARNING, & GENETICS
"Consciousness is meaningless without memory," said
Tim Tully, PhD, a Professor from the Cold Spring Harbor
Laboratory, in New York. Therefore, he speculated, memory
enhancement may be a potentially effective treatment for
various forms of cognitive dysfunction.
From a
neurogenetic perspective, learning and memory impairment may
be caused by defects in the genome. In order to identify the
comprehensive network of genes involved in memory formation,
Dr. Tully studied fruit flies. The Drosophilagenome, he
explained, has been completely mapped and the identification
of memory genes in flies could lead to the identification of
human homologs.
Using
olfactory conditioning as a model for memory and learning, Dr.
Tully and colleagues demonstrated that flies could develop a
"photographic memory" simply by upregulating the cyclic
AMP-response element binding (CREB) protein. Cyclic AMP
metabolism is thought to have a critical role in the
consolidation of short-term changes in neuronal activity into
long-term memory storage in a variety of systems ranging from
the gill and siphon withdrawal reflex in Aplysiato
spatial and contextual learning in mice. Although
extrapolation to humans may not be straightforward, Dr. Tully
suspects that human memory could eventually be reduced to the
expression of genes that fine-tune the neural circuitry
underlying consciousness.
NR
—Andrew Nathan Wilner, MD
Suggested
Reading 1. Damasio AR. Descartes' Error: Emotion,
Reason, and the Human Brain.New York: Grosset/Putnam;
1994. 2. Gazzaniga MS. Cerebral specialization and
interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum
enable the human condition?
Brain.2000;123:1293-1326. 3. Iacoboni M, Woods RP,
Brass M, et al. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation.
Science.1999;286:2526-2528. 4. Mazziotta JC.
Imaging: window on the brain. Arch
Neurol.2000;57:1413-1421.
Return to
table of contents |
|